Sleep & sporting performance
Written by Yann Le Meur and Christophe Hausswirth, France
01-Oct-2014
Category: Sports Medicine

Volume 4 | Targeted Topic - Post | 2015
Volume 4 - Targeted Topic - Post-Exercise Recovery

- Written by Yann Le Meur (@YLMSportScience) and Christophe Hausswirth (@HausswirthC), France

 

Many high-performance athletes are involved in regular, intense training programmes aimed at to stimulating their psychophysiological adaptations in order to improve their capacity for performance. Intense training maximises the role of recovery both in the short-term, between training sessions1, and in the medium-term, for example during tapering phases2. In the longer-term, sleep also appears to play an important role in preventing the occurrence of overreaching or even overtraining syndrome3. it is clear that sleep plays a major role in recovery, which explains why it is considered to be a key factor in sporting success4.

 

SLEEP QUANTITY AND QUALITY IN TRAINED ATHLETES

While sleep is known to contribute to athletes’ recovery, little data is available on the characteristics of sleep in high-performance athletes. Recently, a study carried out at the English Institute of Sport investigated sleep in 47 athletes preparing for the Olympic Games5. These athletes were from various disciplines (canoeing, diving, rowing, speed skating) and their sleep quality and quantity were compared to the same parameters in 20 sedentary subjects over 4 nights. During these 4 nights, subjects were equipped with an actimeter on their wrist. On the whole, the time spent in bed was equivalent for the two groups (8:07±0:20 for the sedentary group vs 8:36±0:53 for the athletes). However, for all the other parameters assessed, sleep quality was poorer in the elite athletes (Table 1).

 

Although these results suggest that high-performance athletes potentially have a lower quality of sleep, the values reported are not within the interval of values recorded for subjects suffering from deep sleep disorders6. However, some specific contexts appear to accentuate this effect. Recently, Sargent et al7 characterised how the quantity and quality of sleep was affected in seven Australian swimmers during an intensive 2-week training camp. This training camp was part of the athletes’ preparation for the Beijing Olympic Games in 2008. Over the 14 days, the swimmers trained on 12 days – starting at 6 am – and had 2 recovery days. On training days, swimmers had an average bedtime of 10.05 pm and woke up at 5.48 am, with an effective sleep time of 5.4 hours. During recovery days, these times were shifted to 12.32 am and 9.47 am, respectively, with an effective sleep duration of 7.1 hours. These results show that during the nights preceding training days, swimmers slept significantly less, thus indicating that starting training early reduces the time athletes spend asleep. Given that sleep restriction, with less than 6 hours of sleep per night, is likely to affect the body’s psychophysiological response capacity, it is possible that starting training too early could hinder adaptation to the training workload.

 

HOW THE TRAINING WORKLOAD AFFECTS SLEEP QUALITY

Despite survey-based health research reporting associations between regular moderate physical activity and better sleep8, a few studies have reported alterations in sleep quality in response to highly demanding training programmes5,9. Taylor et al9 measured sleep using polysomnography during the ‘onset of training’, ‘heavy training’ and ‘pre-competition taper’ training periods in elite female swimmers. Sleep onset latency, time awake after sleep onset, total sleep time, rapid eye movement and deep sleep times were similar during all three training phases. In contrast, the number of movements during sleep was significantly higher (6%) when the training workload was higher, suggesting some effects on sleep. Nevertheless, the improved performance times and low levels of tension and anger at peak training suggest that the swimmers were not in a functionally overreached state in the conditions studied. Recently, Fietze et al10 used wrist actigraphy to study sleep patterns in 24 classical ballet dancers during a 67-day period of high physical and mental stress before a ballet premiere performance. Their results showed a small but significant reduction in sleep duration (-6%), in sleep efficiency (-2%), in time spent in bed (-3%) and an increase in wakefulness after sleep onset (+3%); sleep onset latency was not affected. Nevertheless, these authors did not report changes in physical performance in response to the overload programme prescribed. This makes it difficult to draw clear conclusions on sleep disruption in overreached athletes. Previous studies have, however, reported signs of decreased sleep quality in overreached or overtrained endurance athletes. For example, Jurimaë et al11 monitored the recovery-stress state in competitive male rowers over a 6-day training camp in response to an approximately 100% increase in training load compared to average weekly loads. Using the Recovery-Stress-Questionnaire for Athletes (RESTQ-Sport), these authors showed decreased levels of perceived sleep quality, suggesting that recovery may not have been adequate during this training camp12. This could lead to performance impairment and generate a high level of perceived fatigue (i.e. overreaching). However, without objective markers such as sleep actimetry, it is difficult to associate sleep disruption with overloading based on perceived sleep quality alone. In a similar vein, Matos et al13 recently reported that difficulty sleeping was one of the physical symptoms most frequently reported by athletes. These athletes also experienced persistent daily fatigue and a significant decrease in performance lasting for long periods of time. Altogether, these results suggest that heavy training workloads may have a negative effect on sleep quality, but to date a limited number of studies have provided objective data on changes in sleep characteristics during periods of confirmed functional overreaching. Overall, among the few studies during which sleep was monitored in athletes who demonstrated clear signs of overreaching (i.e. high level of perceived fatigue and decreased performance), most involve self-reporting of subjective parameters such as perceived sleep quality11,14. Recently, Hausswirth et al15 used nocturnal actimetry to study a group of trained triathletes who completed an overload training programme followed by a 2-week taper period. These athletes developed symptoms of functional overreaching while control counterparts showed no signs of training intolerance. The most important finding of this study indicated a progressive decrease in the indices of sleep quality, alongside small reductions in sleep quantity, during the overload period in the functionally overreached athletes. These effects were progressively reversed during the subsequent taper period.

 

Although these results suggest that functionally overreached athletes do suffer from a modest decrease in quality and quantity of sleep during the overload period, their sleep quality and quantity still remained considerably better than that experienced by patients with sleep disorders16, extreme sleep deprivation17 or by athletes with jet lag18 or after hypoxic exposure19. Halson et al20, in a similar study, reported a greater sleep deficiency during the period leading to overtraining (< 6 hours per night) in a talented female sprint cyclist who developed signs of overtraining (i.e. persistent fatigue and underperformance over a number of months). Nevertheless, we cannot exclude that the moderate changes observed with the functional overreaching during the study by Hausswirth et al15 would not unduly affect performance in elite athletes, where very minor differences in performance can have a large impact on the outcome of competitions21. It also remained unclear during this study whether sleep disturbance was involved in the aetiology of overreaching, or simply a symptom of it. Further research will be required to determine the relationship between sleep, training tolerance and adaptation, especially in athletes developing maladaptation to training (i.e. overreaching, overtraining).

 

All of these results on the physiological effects of lack of sleep confirm the primary role played by sleep in the context of post-exercise recovery. Although a growing number of recovery aids is available to athletes, these data remind us of the primary and irreplaceable role of sleep and show that no recovery method is likely to compensate for a lack of sleep. It thus appears wise to assess sleep quality in athletes to prevent and diagnose the onset of overreaching (Table 2)22.

 

This questionnaire can be answered for the various situations presented, by responding to the following single question:

 

How likely are you to doze off or fall asleep in the following situations, in contrast to just feeling tired?

  • 0: would never doze
  • 1: slight chance of dozing
  • 2: moderate chance of dozing
  • 3: high chance of dozing.

 

A score greater than 10 is generally a indicator of a sleep deficit and should lead the subject to reflect on a reorganisation of their timetable.

 

EFFECTS OF LACK OF SLEEP

Sleep deprivation and performance capacity

Due to an often heavily loaded timetable, the competitions in which they participate, long journeys sometimes associated with these or even the anxiety to which they may be subject, athletes often experience sleep disorders23,24. The effects on the sleep cycle can range from moderate insomnia (2 to 4 hours) to disturbance of the whole night’s sleep. Sleep disorders increase the psychophysiological stress associated with exercise and lead to a prolongation of the period necessary for post-exercise recover17. When this occurs during a period when the training workload is high, or during a competition extending over several days, there is a greater risk of overreaching and the performance level can be compromised25.

 

Various studies, in particular in the 1980s, investigated the effects of sleep deprivation on performance and physical fatigue. These concerned a broad spectrum of sporting activities, from weightlifting to endurance sports through extended sprint events, like the Wingate test. As is often the case, the results reported in by these studies are difficult to compare due to the range of different experimental protocols used (duration of sleep deprivation, type of exercise). To simplify matters, we can distinguish between high intensity exercises (jumps, sprints, throws) and prolonged efforts. Overall, high intensity exercises are not overly affected by 1 or 2 sleepless nights26-29, but with longer durations of deprivation (64 hours), explosive strength and dynamic (isokinetic) strength does drop slightly30. The same observation applies to cycling sprints; laboratory performances are not altered by a single sleepless night29,31-34. However, they do decline if intensity and duration are combined. For example, if subjects are asked to repeat sprints every minute for 1 hour, a regression is observed17. Duration is therefore a key parameter in wakefulness during the night. This is confirmed in the context of predominantly aerobic activities35-37. For example, a study by Martin in 198138 showed an 11% drop in performance levels during running exercise performed following 36 hours sleep deprivation. Although athletes do not often encounter these extreme conditions in their daily routine, a deleterious effect of lack of sleep on physical performance has also been reported for lesser perturbations. For example, it appears that a drop in the duration of sleep caused by a later bedtime and/or waking earlier can degrade the aerobic performance level after only 2 days of perturbation37.

 

Many theories have been advanced as to the source of this drop in performance: alteration of the cardiorespiratory system (Table 3), reduced endurance of respiratory muscles39, later sweating40, increased metabolic acidosis41, reduced efficacy of enzymes involved in aerobic metabolism42, reduced yield. All these phenomena have been observed, but we do not know which are truly influential. Some researchers suggest that the explanation may simply be psychological. Thus, the drop in aerobic performance as a result of sleep deprivation could be linked to a lower tolerance to exercise. In support of this, we have observed an increased level of perceived effort after 1 or 2 sleepless nights29,33,38,43. However, other studies appear to contradict this33,44. The increased perceived effort generally results in alterations to sub-maximal performance45,46. The increase in difficulty could also be due to the production of inflammatory substances, which exacerbate pain, during acute sleep deprivation47 or chronic sleep restriction48. Indeed, a study by Martin et al38 indirectly suggests that psychological factors could explain a large part of the drop in performance noted in sleep deprivation conditions as exercise performed at an intensely cold temperature minimised the effects of deprivation. According to these authors, the stress related to a competition would have the same effect as cold and lack of sleep should not hinder performance in a major championship. This conclusion is undoubtedly valid for short and intense efforts, but may not apply for endurance efforts49. In the future, new studies should help to answer this question.

 

Effects of lack of sleep on physiological responses

In parallel to the effects of a lack of sleep on the body’s response to exercise, several studies have shown a deleterious effect of sleep deprivation on many other physiological mechanisms involved in post-exercise recovery.

 

Metabolism and energetic recovery

Skein et al17 recently showed that a sleepless night leads to a drop in muscle glycogen stores compared to a control night, even with a normalised carbohydrates intake. These authors suggested that this difference is mainly linked to the energy expended by athletes during their forced wakefulness. Thus, the sleep disturbance in athletes during a period of intense training or a competition is likely to interfere with their performance capacity by affecting recovery of energy stores. In sporting disciplines requiring significant energy expenditure, it is therefore important to ensure that the athletes get enough sleep since depletion of glycogen reserves is a direct cause of the onset of overreaching50.

 

Endocrine response and muscle recovery

Some authors have shown that a lack of sleep can affect an athlete’s recovery by altering their post-exercise endocrine response51. This is because a reduction in sleep duration is associated with an increase in blood cortisol levels and decreased growth hormone release, both of which contribute to creating a catabolic state52. In other words, lack of sleep in athletes contributes to a negative protein balance by reducing muscle protein resynthesis mechanisms and stimulating those resulting in muscle degradation. The medium-term consequences of a recurrent lack of sleep have never, as yet, been investigated, but it is highly likely that it increases the risk of muscle injury by reducing the athlete’s capacity to recover after exercises inducing muscle damage51.

 

Immune defences

The effect of sleep on immune function is, today, well documented and shows that getting enough sleep is essential to preserving the immune defences53. Several studies have shown that a chronic lack of sleep leads to effects on the immune system. It remains, however, to be determined whether these modifications are linked to sleep perturbation per se, to effects on the circadian rhythm of hormonal secretion or whether they represent a response to general stress which itself leads to a lack of sleep54-56. Whatever the cause, it is essential for athletes to maintain the intimate balance between sleep and the immune defences. Indeed, several authors have shown that a prolonged increase in workload can lead to a drop in the immune defences57. Thus, for example, athletes having completed an ultra-endurance event are known to have an increased prevalence of upper respiratory infections due to a state of immune depression during the weeks following the race58. In some circumstances, aggravating factors can enhance the drop in an athlete's immune defences. For example, during one study on military commandoes, Tiollier et al59 showed that salivary immunoglobulin A – which is a good indicator of the body’s defences against infection – drops in response to chronic exposure to altitude (2500 to 3000 m). Similarly, the risk of getting sick is increased in winter due to the circulation of certain viruses which challenge the body’s defence capacities57. Finally, periods during which the training workload is high also frequently lead to a drop in the body’s defences60,61. All these data reinforce the greater importance of sleep to athletes compared to sedentary subjects and suggest that recurrent infections in this population could be a sign of poor sleep hygiene.

 

Effects of lack of sleep on psychological responses

Lack of sleep causes bad humour, irritability, mental fatigue and loss of motivation62. Perturbation of the sleep cycle, triggered for example by going to bed late, leads to greater drowsiness63 and has effects on the emotional make-up. Athletes who lack sleep generally report a greater level of fatigue and an increased feeling of confusion, which coincides with a drop in performance62,64,65 and pain tolerance66. Lack of sleep also negatively affects several cognitive functions. Thus, the more complex the task to be performed, the greater the impact of lack of sleep67. This effect is also observed even for simple and monotonous tasks involving low environmental stimulation68-70. For athletes, we can therefore hypothesise that a lack of sleep is likely to interfere with learning of techniques, as this generally requires frequent repetition of tasks involving precise movements combined with a high attention level to stabilise them. Similarly, learn strategic approaches, which particularly involve attention resources63,71, memorisation63,72 and logical reasoning64,73, are significantly affected by partial sleep deprivation74. Some studies have, in addition, shown sleep-deprived subjects to have a lower capacity to solve complex tasks. This effect appears to be linked to disturbances in the activity of the prefrontal region of the brain, which is particularly involved in decision-making75. Finally, lack of sleep also affects the proprioceptive capacity and thus significantly increases the risk of injuries, such as sprains76.

 

NUTRITION, HYDRATION AND SLEEP

Nutrition and sleep

Several studies have revealed a strong link between nutrition and sleep4,77. It appears that a lack of sleep can affect the secretion of hormones controlling the appetite. Epidemiological studies in particular demonstrated that a chronic lack of sleep affects food choices and that a reduced duration of sleep is associated with metabolic disorders and a greater prevalence of obesity78. In other words, individuals who do not sleep well generally tend to eat poorly. This association has been shown in children79, adolescents80 and adults81. People who sleep poorly therefore show a greater tendency to eat energy-dense foods, to favour foods with a high glycaemic-index or foods rich in fat and to omit fruit and vegetables from their diet. These individuals generally also have a more irregular feeding pattern and tend to eat more frequently between meals.

 

The relationship between nutrition and sleep works both ways. Thus, some nutritional strategies can in turn promote drowsiness and sleep, while others, in contrast, have a stimulating effect. The phases of wakefulness and sleep through which we pass on a daily basis are, indeed, controlled by neuro-hormonal mechanisms which can be modulated by what we eat.

 

Several macronutrients affect sleep quality, in particular tryptophan. This amino acid is a precursor of serotonin (a neurotransmitter) and melatonin (a hormone which also acts as a neurotransmitter), both of which promote sleep82,83. The amount of serotonin produced by the brain is influenced by the concentration of free tryptophan present in the central nervous system: the more free tryptophan present in the brain, the greater the rate of serotonin synthesis and the higher the melatonin levels in the brain, both of which promote sleep (Figure 2). One study in particular showed that consuming tryptophan was associated with a 45% reduction in sleep onset latency82. Conversely, Arnulf et al84 showed that after a day following a tryptophan-depleted diet, sleep was more fragmented and there were more numerous periods of rapid eye movement sleep. In seeking to understand the relationship between tryptophan and the diet, some researchers have hypothesised that sleep could be promoted by increasing the amount of tryptophan provided by the diet85. Thus Markus et al85 studied how consuming milk proteins – which are very rich in tryptophan – before going to bed affected attention and vigilance the following morning. Their analysis showed that this dietary approach increased the ratio of free tryptophan to other circulating branched-chain amino acids by 130% compared to a placebo condition. Interestingly, their results indicated that taking tryptophan reduced drowsiness and enhanced cognitive performance the next morning compared to the control conditions.

 

Other studies have shown that the uptake of free tryptophan by the central nervous system is stimulated by a high ratio of plasma tryptophan to other amino acids, such as tyrosine, phenylalanine, leucine, isoleucine, valine and methionine86. In addition, carbohydrates consumption can modify the concentrations of these amino acids thanks to its effect on insulin release. Consuming carbohydrates leads to the removal of other amino acids from the blood and facilitates the entry of tryptophan into the brain, where it can produce a sedative effect (Figure 3)87. An effective means to promote relaxation is thus to consume protein-containing foods, such as milk, in combination with glucose-rich foods, as it is unlikely that eating protein-containing foods alone could induce a high enough tryptophan level to effectively promote sleep. Thus, combining intake of carbohydrates and proteins during the same meal increases the availability of free tryptophan and helps with sleep onset87.

 

Hydration and sleep

Late hyper-hydration and sleep

An athlete’s sleep quality can be affected by the hydration strategies used. Given that it is essential to compensate for the loss of water caused by sweating during training or competition, some athletes may tend to consume large volumes of drinks late in the day4. Although the need to rehydrate is unquestionable, it is important to underline that this type of strategy can affect sleep quality by obliging the athlete to get up to urinate during the night. This is why athletes should be reminded that it is preferable to rehydrate regularly during the day and to consume drinks containing sodium after exercise, rather than trying to compensate for dehydration in one go.

 

Negative effects of alcohol on sleep

Consuming alcohol before going to bed affects sleep quality through two contradictory influences. On the one hand, consuming alcohol 30 to 60 minutes before going to bed reduces sleep onset latency88. This explains why many people think that alcohol improves the quality of sleep. However, beyond this positive effect, consuming alcohol significantly reduces the duration of the phases of deep sleep, during which true recovery takes place89. This is all the more worrying as these effects persist until the alcohol has been fully metabolised by the body. As the blood alcohol level drops at a rate of around 0.1 g.hour-1, it is easy to imagine to what extent sleep quality can be affected during the night following an evening of overindulgence. Several studies have also shown that consuming alcohol causes a rise in heart rate, an increase in respiratory frequency, intestinal disorders and headaches90, all of which are largely likely to affect sleep quality. Given the major role played by quality of sleep on the recovery processes in athletes, it is advisable to limit alcohol consumption, particularly during intense training periods. This recommendation is all the stronger given that consuming alcohol also delays restoration of glycogen reserves, favours dehydration and affects muscular recovery91. In terms of body composition, frequent consumption of alcohol-containing products also leads to an increase in adipose tissue and degrades bone health92.

 

Stimulants and sleep

An individual’s quantity and quality of sleep can be affected by the consumption of stimulating substances found in the daily diet. Caffeine, present especially in coffee, tea, in some popular sodas and in energy drinks can in particular affect sleep, even if there are significant inter-individual differences in terms of sensitivity. Bonnet and Arand93 explain that consuming caffeine in the two hours preceding going to bed (for doses exceeding 100 mg i.e. approximately one or two coffees) can increase sleep onset latency while also reducing the time spent in deep sleep as well as total sleep time. Shilo et al94 have since confirmed this result by showing that melatonin secretion is significantly affected by caffeine consumption. Hindmarch et al95, however, demonstrated that these deleterious effects were mainly observed in people not used to consuming caffeine. On the whole, we can conclude that caffeine consumption should be limited after 6 pm, as otherwise the athlete’s sleep quality may be affected.

 

THE VIRTUES OF THE NAP

In many cultures, the early afternoon is associated with a period of relaxation. Indeed, it is well described that drowsiness increases and physical and cognitive performance drop during this stage of the day96,97. As this drop in performance does not appear to be affected by the composition of the midday meal98, it is generally attributed to the biological rhythm99. The nap is an interesting strategy to take advantage of this period of the day in preparation for the activities which will follow later.

 

Waterhouse et al63 studied how an early-afternoon nap affected performance in athletes suffering from a lack of sleep. In this study, 10 male subjects performed at battery of physical and cognitive tests in two different experimental conditions. In the first condition, participants were tested on their reaction times, short-term memory and sprints after a short night’s sleep (4 hours sleep) with no nap. In the second condition the protocol was similar, but subjects took a 30 minute nap after lunch. The results showed the nap to have a positive effect on the state of perceived alertness and on performance during memorisation and rapid decision-making tests. In contrast, no effect was noted during simple reaction tests. Thus, performance was improved during sprints over 2 and 20 m after a nap compared to the control condition (1.019 vs 1.060 seconds and 3.878 vs 3.971 seconds, during 2 and 20 m sprints, respectively). These results indicate that taking a nap after lunch is an effective strategy to improve an athlete’s performance during the afternoon, particularly when they must rise early in the morning and they have difficulty meeting their sleep needs at night. Other authors have confirmed this data by showing visible benefits of a nap even in individuals getting enough sleep during the previous night100. Thus, having a nap appears to represent a simple and effective recovery strategy in athletes. Unless sleep deprivation is very marked, or during periods of particularly intense training, the nap should remain of short duration (less than 30 minutes), so as not to enter into a deep sleep phase, as this is likely to lead to a lethargic state during the remainder of the day101.

 

PRACTICAL RECOMMENDATIONS TO IMPROVE SLEEP QUALITY

Despite the importance of sleep in restoring physiological and cognitive functions, sleep hours are often sacrificed by athletes with a very full timetable. A lack of sleep can thus be caused by insomnia which occurs when the athlete is anxious or is in the initial stages of overreaching due to a high training workload4. In these circumstances, the best solution is to get a good night’s sleep by adopting habits that will favour sleep onset. The included breakout boxes list some simple strategies that can be used to restore effective sleep.

 

THIS PAPER IS PART OF A SERIES ON ATHLETE RECOVERY WHICH WAS PRESENTED AT THE POST-EXERCISE RECOVERY CONFERENCE AT ASPETAR. TO VIEW A 40 MINUTE Q&A SESSION WITH ALL RECOVERY EXPERTS WHO PRESENTED AT THIS CONFERENCE CLICK HERE

 

Yann Le Meur Ph.D.

Sport Scientist

 

Christophe Hausswirth Ph.D.

Head of the Research Department

Director of the Laboratory of Sport

 

Research Department

Laboratory of Sport, Expertise and Performance – INSEP

Paris, FRANCE

 

The authors are organising the INSEP conference on Heat Stress and Sport Performance in June 2015. For the programme and more information visit the conference website

 

 

References

1.         Barnett A. Using recovery modalities between training sessions in elite athletes: does it help? Sports medicine. 2006;36(9):781-96.

2.         Mujika I, Padilla S. Scientific bases for precompetition tapering strategies. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2003 Jul;35(7):1182-7.

3.         Meeusen R, Duclos M, Gleeson M, Rietjens G, Steinacker J, Urhausen A. Prevention, diagnosis and treatment of the Overtraining Syndrome. Eur J Sport Sci. 2006;6:1-14.

4.         Halson S. Nutrition, sleep and recovery. European Journal of Sports Science. 2008;8(2):119-26.

5.         Leeder J, Glaister M, Pizzoferro K, Dawson J, Pedlar C. Sleep duration and quality in elite athletes measured using wristwatch actigraphy. Journal of sports sciences. 2012;30(6):541-5.

6.         Sadeh A, Hauri PJ, Kripke DF, Lavie P. The role of actigraphy in the evaluation of sleep disorders. Sleep. 1995 May;18(4):288-302.

7.         Sargent C, Halson S, Roach GD. Sleep or swim? Early-morning training severely restricts the amount of sleep obtained by elite swimmers. Eur J Sport Sci.

8.         Driver HS, Taylor SR. Exercise and sleep. Sleep medicine reviews. 2000 Aug;4(4):387-402.

9.         Taylor SR, Rogers GG, Driver HS. Effects of training volume on sleep, psychological, and selected physiological profiles of elite female swimmers. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 1997 May;29(5):688-93.

10.       Fietze I, Strauch J, Holzhausen M, Glos M, Theobald C, Lehnkering H, et al. Sleep quality in professional ballet dancers. Chronobiology international. 2009 Aug;26(6):1249-62.

11.       Jurimae J, Maestu J, Purge P, Jurimae T. Changes in stress and recovery after heavy training in rowers. Journal of science and medicine in sport / Sports Medicine Australia. 2004 Sep;7(3):335-9.

12.       Kellmann M, Kallus KW. Recovery-Stress Questionnaire for Athletes: Human Kinetics; 2001.

13.       Matos NF, Winsley RJ, Williams CA. Prevalence of nonfunctional overreaching/overtraining in young English athletes. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2011 Jul;43(7):1287-94.

14.       Jurimae J, Maestu J, Purge P, Jurimae T, Soot T. Relations among heavy training stress, mood state, and performance for male junior rowers. Perceptual and motor skills. 2002 Oct;95(2):520-6.

15.       Hausswirth C, Louis J, Aubry A, Bonnet G, Duffield R, Le Meur Y. Evidence of Disturbed Sleep and Increased Illness in Overreached Endurance Athletes. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2013 Oct 1.

16.       Sadeh A, Acebo C. The role of actigraphy in sleep medicine. Sleep medicine reviews. 2002 Apr;6(2):113-24.

17.       Skein M, Duffield R, Edge J, Short MJ, Mundel T. Intermittent-sprint performance and muscle glycogen after 30 h of sleep deprivation. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2011 Jul;43(7):1301-11.

18.       Leeder JDC, Gardner AS, Foley S, van Someren K, Pedlar CR. The effect of jet lag on parameters of sleep in elite divers quantified by actigraphy. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2009;41(5):57-8.

19.       Pedlar C, Whyte G, Emegbo S, Stanley N, Hindmarch I, Godfrey R. Acute sleep responses in a normobaric hypoxic tent. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2005 Jun;37(6):1075-9.

20.       Halson S, Martin DT, Gardner AS, Fallon K, Gulbin J. Persistent fatigue in a female sprint cyclist after a talent-transfer initiative. International journal of sports physiology and performance. 2006 Mar;1(1):65-9.

21.       Paton C, Hopkins WG. Competitive performance of elite Olympic-Distance triathletes: reliability and smallest worthwhile enhancement. Sportscience. 2005;9:1-5.

22.       Urhausen A, Kindermann W. Diagnosis of overtraining: what tools do we have? Sports medicine. 2002;32(2):95-102.

23.       Bishop D. The effects of travel on team performance in the Australian national netball competition. Journal of science and medicine in sport / Sports Medicine Australia. 2004 Mar;7(1):118-22.

24.       Richmond L, Dawson B, Hillman DR, Eastwood PR. The effect of interstate travel on sleep patterns of elite Australian Rules footballers. Journal of science and medicine in sport / Sports Medicine Australia. 2004 Jun;7(2):186-96.

25.       Kennedy MD, Tamminen KA, Holt NL. Factors that influence fatigue status in Canadian university swimmers. Journal of sports sciences. 2012 Nov 9.

26.       Blumert PA, Crum AJ, Ernsting M, Volek JS, Hollander DB, Haff EE, et al. The acute effects of twenty-four hours of sleep loss on the performance of national-caliber male collegiate weightlifters. Journal of strength and conditioning research / National Strength & Conditioning Association. 2007 Nov;21(4):1146-54.

27.       Copes K, Rosentswieg J. The effects of sleep deprivation upon motor performance of ninth-grade students. The Journal of sports medicine and physical fitness. 1972 Mar;12(1):47-53.

28.       Martin BJ. Sleep deprivation and exercise. Exercise and sport sciences reviews. 1986;14:213-29.

29.       Symons JD, VanHelder T, Myles WS. Physical performance and physiological responses following 60 hours of sleep deprivation. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 1988 Aug;20(4):374-80.

30.       Takeuchi L, Davis GM, Plyley M, Goode R, Shephard RJ. Sleep deprivation, chronic exercise and muscular performance. Ergonomics. 1985 Mar;28(3):591-601.

31.       Mougin F, Bourdin H, Simon-Rigaud ML, Didier JM, Toubin G, Kantelip JP. Effects of a selective sleep deprivation on subsequent anaerobic performance. International journal of sports medicine. 1996 Feb;17(2):115-9.

32.       Souissi N, Bessot N, Chamari K, Gauthier A, Sesboue B, Davenne D. Effect of time of day on aerobic contribution to the 30-s Wingate test performance. Chronobiology international. 2007;24(4):739-48.

33.       Myles WS. Sleep deprivation, physical fatigue, and the perception of exercise intensity. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 1985 Oct;17(5):580-4.

34.       Hill DW, Borden DO, Darnaby KM, Hendricks DN. Aerobic and anaerobic contributions to exhaustive high-intensity exercise after sleep deprivation. Journal of sports sciences. 1994 Oct;12(5):455-61.

35.       Martin BJ. Effect of sleep deprivation on tolerance of prolonged exercise. European journal of applied physiology and occupational physiology. 1981;47(4):345-54.

36.       Martin BJ, Chen HI. Sleep loss and the sympathoadrenal response to exercise. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 1984;16(1):56-9.

37.       Mougin F, Bourdin H, Simon-Rigaud ML, Nguyen NU, Kantelip JP, Davenne D. Hormonal responses to exercise after partial sleep deprivation and after a hypnotic drug-induced sleep. Journal of sports sciences. 2001 Feb;19(2):89-97.

38.       Martin BJ, Gaddis GM. Exercise after sleep deprivation. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 1981;13(4):220-3.

39.       Chen HI, Tang YR. Sleep loss impairs inspiratory muscle endurance. The American review of respiratory disease. 1989 Oct;140(4):907-9.

40.       Dewasmes G, Bothorel B, Hoeft A, Candas V. Regulation of local sweating in sleep-deprived exercising humans. European journal of applied physiology and occupational physiology. 1993;66(6):542-6.

41.       Chen HI. Effects of 30-h sleep loss on cardiorespiratory functions at rest and in exercise. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 1991 Feb;23(2):193-8.

42.       Vondra K, Brodan V, Bass A, Kuhn E, Teisinger J, Andel M, et al. Effects of sleep deprivation on the activity of selected metabolic enzymes in skeletal muscle. European journal of applied physiology and occupational physiology. 1981;47(1):41-6.

43.       Oliver SJ, Costa RJ, Laing SJ, Bilzon JL, Walsh NP. One night of sleep deprivation decreases treadmill endurance performance. European journal of applied physiology. 2009 Sep;107(2):155-61.

44.       McMurray RG, Fafrowicz JF, Berry M. The endorphin response of women to sleep loss and exercise. Aviation, space, and environmental medicine. 1988 Feb;59(2):129-32.

45.       Marcora SM, Staiano W, Manning V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. Journal of applied physiology. 2009 Mar;106(3):857-64.

46.       Millet GY. Can neuromuscular fatigue explain running strategies and performance in ultra-marathons?: the flush model. Sports medicine. 2011 Jun 1;41(6):489-506.

47.       Vgontzas AN, Papanicolaou DA, Bixler EO, Lotsikas A, Zachman K, Kales A, et al. Circadian interleukin-6 secretion and quantity and depth of sleep. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 1999 Aug;84(8):2603-7.

48.       Haack M, Sanchez E, Mullington JM. Elevated inflammatory markers in response to prolonged sleep restriction are associated with increased pain experience in healthy volunteers. Sleep. 2007 Sep;30(9):1145-52.

49.       Millet GY. Les nuits de la pleine lune. Sport et Vie. 2012;130:10-7.

50.       Costill DL, Flynn MG, Kirwan JP, Houmard JA, Mitchell JB, Thomas R, et al. Effects of repeated days of intensified training on muscle glycogen and swimming performance. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 1988 Jun;20(3):249-54.

51.       VanHelder T, Radomski MW. Sleep deprivation and the effect on exercise performance. Sports medicine. 1989 Apr;7(4):235-47.

52.       Obal F, Jr., Krueger JM. GHRH and sleep. Sleep medicine reviews. 2004 Oct;8(5):367-77.

53.       Lange T, Dimitrov S, Born J. Effects of sleep and circadian rhythm on the human immune system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010 Apr;1193:48-59.

54.       Kusaka Y, Kondou H, Morimoto K. Healthy lifestyles are associated with higher natural killer cell activity. Preventive medicine. 1992 Sep;21(5):602-15.

55.       Lucey DR, Clerici M, Shearer GM. Type 1 and type 2 cytokine dysregulation in human infectious, neoplastic, and inflammatory diseases. Clinical microbiology reviews. 1996 Oct;9(4):532-62.

56.       Nakachi K, Imai K. Environmental and physiological influences on human natural killer cell activity in relation to good health practices. Japanese journal of cancer research : Gann. 1992 Aug;83(8):798-805.

57.       Walsh NP, Gleeson M, Pyne DB, Nieman DC, Dhabhar FS, Shephard RJ, et al. Position statement. Part two: Maintaining immune health. Exercise immunology review. 2011;17:64-103.

58.       Nieman DC. Exercise and resistance to infection. Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology. 1998 May;76(5):573-80.

59.       Tiollier E, Schmitt L, Burnat P, Fouillot JP, Robach P, Filaire E, et al. Living high-training low altitude training: effects on mucosal immunity. European journal of applied physiology. 2005 Jun;94(3):298-304.

60.       Tiollier E, Gomez-Merino D, Burnat P, Jouanin JC, Bourrilhon C, Filaire E, et al. Intense training: mucosal immunity and incidence of respiratory infections. European journal of applied physiology. 2005 Jan;93(4):421-8.

61.       Morgado JM, Rama L, Silva I, de Jesus Inacio M, Henriques A, Laranjeira P, et al. Cytokine production by monocytes, neutrophils, and dendritic cells is hampered by long-term intensive training in elite swimmers. European journal of applied physiology. 2012 Feb;112(2):471-82.

62.       Meney I, Waterhouse J, Atkinson G, Reilly T, Davenne D. The effect of one night's sleep deprivation on temperature, mood, and physical performance in subjects with different amounts of habitual physical activity. Chronobiology international. 1998 Jul;15(4):349-63.

63.       Waterhouse J, Atkinson G, Edwards B, Reilly T. The role of a short post-lunch nap in improving cognitive, motor, and sprint performance in participants with partial sleep deprivation. Journal of sports sciences. 2007 Dec;25(14):1557-66.

64.       Bonnet MH. Sleep, performance and mood after the energy-expenditure equivalent of 40 hours of sleep deprivation. Psychophysiology. 1980 Jan;17(1):56-63.

65.       Reilly T, Piercy M. The effect of partial sleep deprivation on weight-lifting performance. Ergonomics. 1994 Jan;37(1):107-15.

66.       Onen SH, Alloui A, Gross A, Eschallier A, Dubray C. The effects of total sleep deprivation, selective sleep interruption and sleep recovery on pain tolerance thresholds in healthy subjects. Journal of sleep research. 2001 Mar;10(1):35-42.

67.       Ikegami K, Ogyu S, Arakomo Y, Suzuki K, Mafune K, Hiro H, et al. Recovery of cognitive performance and fatigue after one night of sleep deprivation. Journal of occupational health. 2009;51(5):412-22.

68.       Rosekind MR. Sleep medicine: past lessons, present challenges, and future opportunities. Sleep medicine reviews. 2008 Aug;12(4):249-51.

69.       Van Dongen HP, Dinges DF. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and psychomotor vigilance. Clinics in sports medicine. 2005 Apr;24(2):237-49, vii-viii.

70.       Waterhouse J, Folkard S, Van Dongen H, Minors D, Owens D, Kerkhof G, et al. Temperature profiles, and the effect of sleep on them, in relation to morningness-eveningness in healthy female subjects. Chronobiology international. 2001 Mar;18(2):227-47.

71.       Scott JP, McNaughton LR, Polman RC. Effects of sleep deprivation and exercise on cognitive, motor performance and mood. Physiology & behavior. 2006 Feb 28;87(2):396-408.

72.       Kim DJ, Lee HP, Kim MS, Park YJ, Go HJ, Kim KS, et al. The effect of total sleep deprivation on cognitive functions in normal adult male subjects. The International journal of neuroscience. 2001 Jul;109(1-2):127-37.

73.       Angus RG, Heslegrave RJ, Myles WS. Effects of prolonged sleep deprivation, with and without chronic physical exercise, on mood and performance. Psychophysiology. 1985 May;22(3):276-82.

74.       Brawn TP, Fenn KM, Nusbaum HC, Margoliash D. Consolidation of sensorimotor learning during sleep. Learning & memory. 2008 Nov;15(11):815-9.

75.       Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. Journal of experimental psychology Applied. 2000 Sep;6(3):236-49.

76.       Ivins D. Acute ankle sprain: an update. American family physician. 2006 Nov 15;74(10):1714-20.

77.       Peuhkuri K, Sihvola N, Korpela R. Diet promotes sleep duration and quality. Nutrition research. 2012 May;32(5):309-19.

78.       Morselli L, Leproult R, Balbo M, Spiegel K. Role of sleep duration in the regulation of glucose metabolism and appetite. Best practice & research Clinical endocrinology & metabolism. 2010 Oct;24(5):687-702.

79.       Moreira P, Santos S, Padrao P, Cordeiro T, Bessa M, Valente H, et al. Food patterns according to sociodemographics, physical activity, sleeping and obesity in Portuguese children. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2010 Mar;7(3):1121-38.

80.       Weiss A, Xu F, Storfer-Isser A, Thomas A, Ievers-Landis CE, Redline S. The association of sleep duration with adolescents' fat and carbohydrate consumption. Sleep. 2010 Sep;33(9):1201-9.

81.       Shi Z, McEvoy M, Luu J, Attia J. Dietary fat and sleep duration in Chinese men and women. International journal of obesity. 2008 Dec;32(12):1835-40.

82.       Hartmann E. Effects of L-tryptophan on sleepiness and on sleep. Journal of psychiatric research. 1982;17(2):107-13.

83.       Hartmann E, Spinweber CL. Sleep induced by L-tryptophan. Effect of dosages within the normal dietary intake. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 1979 Aug;167(8):497-9.

84.       Arnulf I, Quintin P, Alvarez JC, Vigil L, Touitou Y, Lebre AS, et al. Mid-morning tryptophan depletion delays REM sleep onset in healthy subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002 Nov;27(5):843-51.

85.       Markus CR, Jonkman LM, Lammers JH, Deutz NE, Messer MH, Rigtering N. Evening intake of alpha-lactalbumin increases plasma tryptophan availability and improves morning alertness and brain measures of attention. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2005 May;81(5):1026-33.

86.       Wurtman RJ, Wurtman JJ, Regan MM, McDermott JM, Tsay RH, Breu JJ. Effects of normal meals rich in carbohydrates or proteins on plasma tryptophan and tyrosine ratios. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2003 Jan;77(1):128-32.

87.       Afaghi A, O'Connor H, Chow CM. High-glycemic-index carbohydrate meals shorten sleep onset. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2007 Feb;85(2):426-30.

88.       MacLean AW, Cairns J. Dose-response effects of ethanol on the sleep of young men. Journal of studies on alcohol. 1982 May;43(5):434-44.

89.       Rundell OH, Lester BK, Griffiths WJ, Williams HL. Alcohol and sleep in young adults. Psychopharmacologia. 1972;26(3):201-18.

90.       Feige B, Gann H, Brueck R, Hornyak M, Litsch S, Hohagen F, et al. Effects of alcohol on polysomnographically recorded sleep in healthy subjects. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2006 Sep;30(9):1527-37.

91.       Burke L. Nutrition for recovery after training and competition. In: Burke V, Deakin V, editors. Clinical sports nutrition. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2006. p. 415–47.

92.       Maddalozzo GF, Turner RT, Edwards CH, Howe KS, Widrick JJ, Rosen CJ, et al. Alcohol alters whole body composition, inhibits bone formation, and increases bone marrow adiposity in rats. Osteoporosis international : a journal established as result of cooperation between the European Foundation for Osteoporosis and the National Osteoporosis Foundation of the USA. 2009 Sep;20(9):1529-38.

93.       Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Caffeine use as a model of acute and chronic insomnia. Sleep. 1992;15:526-36.

94.       Shilo L, Sabbah H, Hadari R, Kovatz S, Weinberg U, Dolev S, et al. The effects of coffee consumption on sleep and melatonin secretion. Sleep medicine. 2002 May;3(3):271-3.

95.       Hindmarch I, Rigney U, Stanley N, Quinlan P, Rycroft J, Lane J. A naturalistic investigation of the effects of day-long consumption of tea, coffee and water on alertness, sleep onset and sleep quality. Psychopharmacology. 2000 Apr;149(3):203-16.

96.       Dinges DF. Napping patterns and effects in human adults. In: RJE B, editor. Sleep and Alertness: Chronobiological, Behavioral, and Medical Aspects of Napping. New York: Raven Press; 1989. p. 171-204.

97.       Dinges DF. Adult napping and its effects on ability to function. In: CE S, editor. Why We Nap: Evolution, Chronobiology, and Functions of Polyphasic and Ultrashort Sleep. Boston: Birkhäuser; 1992. p. 118-34.

98.       Stahl ML, Orr WC, Bollinger C. Postprandial sleepiness: objective documentation via polysomnography. Sleep. 1983;6(1):29-35.

99.       Broughton RJ. Chronobiological aspects and models of sleep and napping.  Sleep and Alertness: Chronobiological, Behavioral, and Medical Aspects of Napping. New York: Raven Press; 1989. p. 71-98.

100.     Takahashi M, Nakata A, Haratani T, Ogawa Y, Arito H. Post-lunch nap as a worksite intervention to promote alertness on the job. Ergonomics. 2004;47:1003-13.

101.     Horne JA, Reyner LA. Counteracting driver sleepiness: effects of napping, caffeine, and placebo. Psychophysiology. 1996 May;33(3):306-9.

102         Peuhkuri K, Sihvola N, Korpela R. Diet promotes sleep duration and quality. Nutr Res. 2012;32(5):309-19.

103         Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 1991; 14:540-545

 

Image via Kris Krug

Sleep actigraphy data comparing athletes with non-athletic controls. Values are means ± standard deviation. *Significantly different from control. Adapted from Leeder et al5.
Assessing the degree of daytime sleepiness using the Epworth questionnaire103
Main modifications induced by a lack of sleep on the functioning of the cardiorespiratory system. Adapted from Millet49.
Tips for effective sleep.
Tips for effective sleep.
Influences of the nutrients provided by the diet on serotonin and melatonin synthesis. A) protein intake directly contributes to increasing the amount of free TRP in the blood. B) carbohydrate intake, by triggering insulin release, contributes to the uptake of TRP into the brain, by favouring the uptake of other amino acids by other cells. C) the increased TRP concentration in the brain leads to an increase in serotonin and melatonin synthesis, which promotes sleep. Adapted from Peukhuri et al102. TRP=tryptophan, LNAA=large neutral amino acids, TDO= tryptophan 2,3-dioxygenase, NAT= arylalkylamine-N-acetyltransferase, AADC= aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase, 5-HTP=5-hydroxytryptophan.
Tips for effective sleep.
Tips for effective sleep.
Tips for effective sleep.
Tips for effective sleep.
Tips for effective sleep.

Share

Volume 4 | Targeted Topic - Post | 2015
Volume 4 - Targeted Topic - Post-Exercise Recovery

More from Aspetar Journal

Sports Medicine
Recovery nutrition for the Arabic athlete competing in team based sports

Written by – Richard Allison and Shaima Al Khaldi, Qatar

Sports Medicine
Electrostimulation recovery-related strategies

Written by – Nicola A. Maffiuletti, Switzerland and Grégory Dupont, France

Interview
Taoufik Makhloufi

Interview by – Dr Juan Manuel Alonso, Qatar

Latest Issue

Download Volume 13 - Targeted Topic - Extreme Sports Medicine | 2024

Trending

Editorial
From our editor
Editorial
FROM OUR GUEST EDITORS
Interview
Dr Andrew Murray
Letters From
A LETTER FROM THE HEART
Sports Medicine
WHERE HUMANITY GOES, MEDICINE MUST FOLLOW

Categories

Member of
Organization members